Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Score reliability and factor similarity of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-3 (SATAQ-3) among four ethnic groups

Abstract

Background

This study evaluated the score reliability and equivalence of factor structure of the Sociocultural Attitudes towards Appearance Questionnaire-3 (SATAQ-3) [1] in a sample of female college students from the four largest ethnic groups in the USA.

Methods

Participants were 1245 women who self-identified as European American/White (n = 543), African American/Black (n = 137), Asian American (n = 317), or Latina/Hispanic (n = 248). All completed the SATAQ-3 and a demographic questionnaire. To test the factor similarity and score reliability across groups, we used exploratory factor analysis and calculated Cronbach’s alphas (respectively).

Results

Score reliability was high for all groups. Tests of factor equivalence suggested that the four pre-established factors of the SATAQ-3 (i.e., knowledge, perceived pressure, thin-ideal internalization, athletic-ideal internalization) were similar for women of all ethnic groups. Only two items (20 and 27) did not consistently load on the previously identified scale across all four groups. When scored, African Americans reported significantly less perceived pressure and internalization than all other groups.

Conclusions

Results support the use of the SATAQ-3 in female college students of these four ethnicities.

Background

Images of the ideal-looking woman inundate mainstream Western media [2]. Through film, advertisements, television shows, music videos, and magazines, the ideal woman in Western cultures (e.g., in mainstream USA, Australia, Western Europe) is depicted as thin, young, tall, and fit with long hair, a thin waist and fair skin [3, 4]. For example, Spitzer, Henderson and Zivian [4] found that nearly all Playboy and Miss America pageant contestants from 1977 to 1996 were underweight, with 17-33% meeting diagnostic weight criteria for anorexia nervosa. In a similar study, Voracek and Fisher [5] found that Playboy centerfold models’ bust size, hip size, and waist-to-hip ratio decreased significantly from 1950’s to the 1990’s.

In addition to idealizing and valuing a specific physique, mainstream Western culture and media promote the message that attaining the ideal appearance assures social status, happiness, success, and personal worth for women [68]. For example, Stokes and Frederick-Recascino [8] found significant positive correlations between happiness and perceptions of sexual desirability, weight satisfaction, and physical fitness among adult women. Furthermore, Western cultures’ emphasis on individualism, self-reliance, and competitiveness makes women responsible for maintaining a low weight and an ‘attractive’ physical appearance [6, 911]. Combined, cultural values and ideals of appearance promoted by mainstream Western media emphasize that attaining a thin, fit body is fundamental to women’s social value and moral character.

Media exposure and eating pathology

Conceptually, values and ideals of appearance promoted in mainstream Western media are etiologically tied to the development of eating pathology through a progression of knowledge, perceived pressure, and internalization [1, 12, 13]. As women are exposed to Western media, they develop increased information or awareness of cultural values and ideals of appearance. As such awareness increases, women often report increased pressure to attain the ideal and internalize these ideals by adopting them as self-relevant and behaviorally try to attain them (e.g., through dieting, exercise, make-up). For women, internalization often manifests in a desire to look like very thin or fit models in the media (i.e., thin- and athletic-ideal internalization, respectively).

A growing body of research supports this model and the role of internalization of Western sociocultural values and ideals of appearance on eating pathology in women [12, 1420]. In a longitudinal study of 216 children ages 6–14, thin-ideal internalization prospectively predicted increased body dissatisfaction, depressive symptoms, preoccupation with thinness, and a larger perceived body size for girls [21]. Similarly, a meta-analysis of cross-sectional data examining risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology found a medium to large association between thin-ideal internalization and body dissatisfaction in women [22]. Although less studied to date, research also suggests that athletic-ideal internalization is associated with increased compulsive exercise [23] and body dissatisfaction [24, 25] in women.

Measurement of sociocultural attitudes towards appearance and ethnicity

Given the strong relationship between internalization of mainstream Western media ideals and eating pathology [1217, 21, 22, 2426], it is critical for researchers and clinicians to have psychometrically sound assessment tools measuring these constructs. The Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-3 (SATAQ-3) [1] is one of the most commonly used self-report measures of endorsement of Western appearance ideals. A 30-item questionnaire with four subscales that correspond with the theoretical model described above (i.e., information, perceived pressure, thin- and athletic-ideal internalization), the SATAQ-3 has been used in many populations including patients with eating disorders [27, 28], adolescents [2932], college students [33, 34], and community samples [3537].

Despite the widespread use of the SATAQ-3, data on its administration to different racial/ethnic groups in the USA are sparse. Recently, Franko and colleagues [38] found that the SATAQ-3 demonstrated adequate score reliability and validity among Latina college students. However, few data exist on the psychometric properties of the SATAQ-3 among African American/Black and Asian American female college students. This is problematic because research indicates considerable ethnic differences in appearance ideals and eating pathology [3942]. For example, in a meta-analysis of body dissatisfaction among European American/White, African American/Black, Asian American, and Latina women, Grabe and Hyde [40] found that European American/White women reported higher body dissatisfaction than African American/Black women. Nouri, Hill, and Orrell-Valente [43] found similar levels of thin-ideal internalization and body dissatisfaction among European American and Asian American college females. However, a study examining body dissatisfaction among Asian subgroups [44] found that Chinese females reported low body- and self-dissatisfaction, whereas Japanese females reported high body- and self-dissatisfaction (despite similarly low body size in both groups). As such, the relationship between ethnicity and endorsement of Western media is complex and may influence the utility of the SATAQ-3 in various ethnic groups.

Current study

Given the importance of studying the influence of Western cultural ideals promoted by the media on eating pathology in different cultural and ethnic samples, this study evaluated the score reliability and equivalence of factor structure of the SATAQ-3 in a sample of European American/White, African American/Black, Asian American, and Hispanic/Latina female college students in the USA. Consistent with previous research, we predicted that the SATAQ-3 would have good psychometric properties in female college students of all ethnic groups.

Method

Participants

Data from 1245 American female college students in the southwest USA were selected for this study from a large ongoing study investigating eating pathology in female college students. Women were recruited from introductory psychology classes between 2010 and 2012 and all received research credit in exchange for study participation. Although any woman could participate in the larger study, only women who self-identified as a member of one of the four ethnic groups of interest (N = 1347 women) with no missing data were included in this study (which excluded 102 participants). This yielded a total sample of European American/White (n = 543; 43.6%), African American/Black (n = 137; 11.0%), Asian American (n = 317; 25.5%), or Latina (n = 248; 19.9%) women with complete data.

Measures

Demographics

Participants reported their gender, age, weight, height, ethnicity, and generational status.

Sociocultural attitudes toward appearance

As discussed above, the SATAQ-3 [1] is a 30-item measure of endorsement of Western sociocultural beauty standards depicted in the media. Items are rated on a five point response scale ranging from definitely disagree (1) to definitely agree (5), with higher scores indicating greater media endorsement. According to the original validation by Thompson and colleagues [1], the SATAQ-3 has four factors/subscales: Information (INFO), Perceived Pressure (PRESS), Internalization-General (INT-GEN), and Internalization-Athlete (INT-ATH). Specifically, INFO consists of 9 items measuring recognition of the social importance of the media’s messages about Western beauty ideals (e.g., “Magazine advertisements are an important source of information about fashion and "being attractive”); PRESS consists of 7 items assessing perceived feelings of pressure to conform to the Western ideals exhibited by the media (e.g., “I've felt pressure from TV or magazines to have a perfect body”); INT-GEN consists of 9 items measuring internalization of the thin-ideal (e.g., “I compare my body to the bodies of people who are on TV” and “I wish I looked like the models in music videos”); and INT-ATH consists of 5 items measuring internalization of the athletic ideal (e.g., "I compare my body to that of people in "good shape" and “I try to look like sports athletes”). Previous research suggests that the SATAQ-3 has high internal consistency in female undergraduates [1] and patients with eating disorders [27].

Statistical analyses

For all statistical analyses, we used IBM SPSS Statistics version 19. In addition to examining descriptive information, we calculated Cronbach’s alphas for the total score and each subscale score based on the factor structure identified by Thompson and colleagues [1] to measure score reliability. To examine the factor structure of the SATAQ-3, we first examined the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy (MSA) [45] to ensure that the data were appropriate for factor analysis. According to Kaiser, MSA values in the .80 - .90 range or higher are ideal and values greater than .70 are adequate. We then conducted an exploratory factor analysis with principal axis extraction (as originally done by Thompson et al. [1]) and oblimen (oblique) rotation separately for each group. We specified a four-factor model because of the established structure of the SATAQ-3, but also examined how well that fit the data.

We then tested the similarity of the factor structure across the four groups using Tucker’s congruence coefficient [46], which is one way to examine factor similarity of measures with complex structure across different samples [47, 48]. Llorente, Warren, Lourdes Pérez de Eulate and Gleaves [49] and Wilksch and Wade [50] recently used this method to examine the equivalence of the factor structure the SATAQ-3 across different groups. Although confirmatory factor analysis is sometimes used in such situations, Lorenzo-Seva and ten Burge [46] noted that “exploratory factor analysis (EFA) might be more appropriate as a basis for factor comparisons than the CFA approach in most applications, especially in large multidimensional solutions that do not approach very simple structures.” Lorenzo-Seva and ten Burge [46] found that values for congruence coefficients ranging from .85–.94 indicate fair similarity and values higher than .95 indicate that two factors can be considered equal.

Results

Descriptive analyses

As shown in Table 1, participants averaged about 21 years of age (range = 18 to 64) and were in the average weight range [51]. ANOVA’s indicated that ethnic groups differed significantly on age [F (3, 1201) = 7.42, p < .01] and BMI [F (3, 1221) = 9.63, p < .01] such that Asian Americans were significantly younger than both European Americans and African Americans; and Asian Americans had a significantly lower BMI than all other groups.

Table 1 Means (SDs) and ANOVAs examining demographic and outcome variables by ethnic group

There were statistically significant ethnic differences on all of the SATAQ-3 subscales when examined according to the original scoring by Thompson and colleagues [1]: INFO, F (3, 1241) = 4.54, p < .01; PRESS, F (3, 1241) = 11.44, p < .01; INT-GEN, F (3, 1241) = 21.04, p < .01; INT-ATH, F (3, 1241) = 17.52, p < .01. As displayed in Table 1, Asian Americans reported significantly more knowledge of Western media ideals (INFO) than European Americans; African Americans reported significantly less perceived pressure (PRESS) and thin-ideal internalization (INT-GEN) than all other groups; European Americans reported significantly more thin-ideal internalization than African Americans and Latinas; European Americans reported more athletic-ideal internalization (INT-ATH) than all other groups; and, African Americans reported less athletic-ideal internalization than all other groups.

Internal consistency and sampling adequacy

In all four groups, score reliability was high and generally equivalent: Cronbach’s alphas for the total score were .97 for each of the four groups. When examined by subscale, INFO, PRESS, and INT-GEN were above .95 and INT-ATH was above .89 for all ethnic groups (see Table 2). MSAs by group were also very similar and good (MSA = .96 for European Americans; .93 for African Americans; .96 for Latinas; and .96 for Asian Americans). We also examined MSAs for individual items to see whether there were any problematic items in any of the groups: All were above .90 for European Americans, Asian Americans, and Latinas and .86 for African Americans. Thus, all of the datasets were clearly suitable for factor analysis.

Table 2 Congruence coefficients and Cronbach’s alpha values (α) by subscale and ethnicity

Examination of factor structures

For the factor analyses, we specified a four-factor model because of the previously identified factor structure by Thompson and colleagues [1]. However, we also examined the degree to which a four-factor model seemed appropriate. For European Americans, there were four factors with initial eigenvalues greater than 1.0 followed by a clear drop-off; thus a four-factor solution appeared appropriate, which explained 74.49% of the total variance. For African Americans, there were three factors with eigenvalues of greater than 1.0; however the fourth was just below 1.0 (0.96) and there was a more clear drop-off after that. Thus, a four-factor solution, which explained 74.66% of the total variance, also seemed appropriate for this group. For Latinas, there were four factors that accounted for 74.54% of the total variance, and there was a clear drop-off between the fourth and fifth factors; thus, four factors again seemed appropriate. Finally, for Asian Americans, there were four eigenvalues greater than 1.0 and a clear drop-off after that. The four extracted factors explained 72.89% of the total variance.

Rotated factor loadings (from the factor pattern matrix) by ethnic group and previously identified subscale (i.e., INFO, PRESS, INT-GEN, and INT-ATH) are depicted in Table 3. Factor loadings of .40 or greater, which we used as the minimum criterion for determining if an item loaded on a particular factor, appear in bold. We used .40 because it was the minimum studied in Guadagnoli and Velicer’s 1988 study [52] of the relationship between sample size and factor stability. In most instances, the four previously identified factors were clearly evident and matched the four subscales identified by previous research. However, the factors were not always in the same order. For European Americans and Latinas, Factor 1 was clearly INT-GEN; Factor 2 was INFO; Factor 3 was INT-ATH; and Factor 4 was PRESS. For Asian and African Americans, factors 1 and 4 switched places, whereas 2 and 3 were the same as European and Latinas. Only two items (20 and 27) did not consistently load on the previously identified scale across all four groups.

Table 3 Rotated factor loadings by ethnic group and subscale

Finally, congruence coefficients were almost all in the very high range, which suggests that the factors could be considered equal across the four groups (see Table 2). The only value below .95 was the .91 for the congruence between the African and Asian American groups; of course, that value was still in the “fair similarity” range [46].

Discussion

Overall, the SATAQ-3 exhibited strong psychometric properties in a sample of European American/White, African American/Black, Asian American, and Latina/Hispanic college women from the USA. Cronbach’s alpha values and MSAs were excellent for all four groups; patterns of eigenvalues indicated a four-factor model was appropriate for all groups; and factor analyses showed very similar patterns for European American/White, Latina, and Asian American women. These results are consistent with previous psychometric evaluations of the SATAQ-3 in female college samples [1, 27].

That said, items 20 (“I compare my body to that of people in ‘good shape’”) and 27 (“I try to look like the people on TV”) did not load as expected for Latina and Asian American women and had very low loadings for the European and African American group. Specifically, item 20 loaded on a different subscale than expected for the Latina and Asian American groups (i.e., INT-GEN instead of INT-ATH, although just barely for the Asian American group) and item 27 did not load higher than .40 on any subscale. Additionally, both of those items had the lowest loadings on the intended subscale for both the European American and African American groups. Similar problems have been noted with item 20 in other studies [27, 37, 53], which lead some researchers to suggest removing the item [30, 53]. Additionally, although less consistently documented in the literature, two recent studies reported low factor loadings for item 27 [30, 53]. To complicate matters, however, item 27 is sometimes negatively worded and was in both studies noted above. Although the original validation study did not include negatively worded items, Thompson and colleagues [1] recommended reverse scoring eight items to test for response bias. Given difficulties reported in previous psychometric investigations with negatively worded items, we did not negatively word any items in this dataset, as suggested by Wheeler and colleagues [37]. However, the lack of consistency in wording could have influenced results for this item.

Additionally, when scored according to the factor structure established by Thompson and colleagues [1], it is interesting to note that African American/Black women reported significantly less perceived pressure, thin-ideal internalization, and athletic-ideal internalization than all other groups. Theoretically, this makes sense as mainstream American media present the ideal woman as thin, young, tall, and predominantly White. In contrast, the ideal female figure in African American/Black culture is curvy with medium sized breasts and large buttocks [54] and more value is placed on personal attributes (e.g., attitude, style) than solely on physical appearance [41, 55, 56]. Consequently, although women of all ethnic groups likely report some appearance-related pressure, African American/Black women may not internalize or feel as much pressure to attain the thin-ideal because it is not as relevant to the ethnic ideal of beauty.

Despite the importance of these findings, they should be considered in light of the study limitations. The sample comprised female college students who self-identified as belonging to one ethnic group, which may limit the generalizability of these findings to other populations (e.g., women of other ages, educational backgrounds, ethnicities). In addition, ethnic groups are highly heterogeneous: by generalizing results within each ethnic group, the study did not test the influence of various intra-group factors that may influence responses such as acculturation, generational status, or ethnic identity [20, 39]. Future research examining the SATAQ-3 would benefit from examining ethnic subgroups (e.g., Korean American) and cultures (e.g., British samples). Finally, a new version of the SATAQ (i.e., the SATAQ-4) is currently being published [57]. Although we believe that the SATAQ-3 will continue to be widely used because of its strong psychometric properties, many linguistic translations, and subscale structure (e.g., the SATAQ-4 only has internalization and pressure subscales), it is a potential limitation. Of note, neither item 20 nor item 27 appears in the SATAQ-4.

Despite these limitations, these results lend support for the use of the SATAQ-3 and its subscales as proposed by Thompson and colleagues [1] among the ethnic groups that we studied. Future research on media internalization with multi-ethnic samples should examine internalization of other beauty ideals (e.g., a curvaceous-ideal). For example, despite having lower levels of thin-ideal internalization in this sample, research suggests that African American/Black women do internalize more ethnically-salient appearance ideals (e.g., a more pronounced buttocks, larger breasts, preferred skin color) and that a discrepancy between the curvaceous-ideal and one’s current body shape is associated with body image problems [54, 58]. For example, Overstreet and colleagues [54] found that Black women preferred a curvier ideal than White women and that dissatisfaction with ones appearance in comparison to the curvaceous ideal predicted appearance-related concerns in Black women. In a sample of 124 African American college females, Falconer and Neville [59] found that greater satisfaction with one’s skin color was associated with more positive body image. Consequently, among African American/Black women, internalization of media depicting a curvaceous-ideal that highlight body areas more salient to African American/Black ideals of beauty (e.g., butt size, skin color, breast size) may be more salient to body image problems in this group. Future research should include a more detailed examination of this possibility and the role of perceived media pressure to attain different appearance ideals (e.g., curvaceous-ideal internalization) among ethnic groups who do not espouse ultra-thin beauty ideals for women.

Conclusions

The purpose of the present study was to evaluate the score reliability and equivalence of factor structure of the SATAQ-3 in a sample of European American, African American, Asian American and Latina female college students. The results lend support to the use of the SATAQ-3 in these four ethnic groups, as evidenced by high score reliability and structure similarity of the four factors (i.e., awareness, perceived pressure, thin-ideal internalization and athletic-ideal internalization). Notably, questionnaire items 20 and 27 did not load consistently on the four-factor scale. In addition, African American participants’ scores on thin-ideal internalization and pressure were significantly lower than all other groups. Future directions include examination of the curvaceous, rather than the thin-ideal, as well as study of ethnic and cultural subgroups.

References

  1. Thompson JK, Van den Berg P, Roehrig M, Guarda AS, Heinberg LJ: The Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Scale-3 (SATAQ-3): development and validation. Int J Eat Disord. 2004, 35: 293-304. 10.1002/eat.10257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Arredondo P, Ivey A, Sue DW, Parham T, Sue DW, Mio JS, Par W, Manese J, Croteau J, Helms J, Horwatt S, Boggs K, Stone G, Perez P, Rice T, Carter J, Burke LP, Trimble J, Vasquez M, Carter R, Casas JM, Pope-davis D, Forrest L, Jensen M, Lafromboise T, Ponterotto JG, Nuttall V, Shullman SL, Caldwell-colbert T, Affairs E: Guidelines on multicultural education, training, research, practice, and organizational change for Psychologists. Am Psychol. 2003, 58: 377-402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Harrison K: Television viewers’ ideal body proportions: the case of the curvaceously thin woman. Sex Roles. 2003, 48: 255-264. 10.1023/A:1022825421647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Spitzer BL, Henderson KA, Zivian MT: Gender differences in population versus media body sizes: A comparison over four decades. Sex Roles. 1999, 40: 545-565. 10.1023/A:1018836029738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Voracek M, Fisher ML: Shapely centrefolds? Temporal change in body measures: trend analysis. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.). 2002, 325: 1447-1448. 10.1136/bmj.325.7378.1447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Katz JH: The sociopolitical nature of counseling. Couns Psychol. 1985, 13: 615-624. 10.1177/0011000085134005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Striegel-Moore RH, Silberstein LR, Rodin J: Toward an understanding of risk factors for bulimia. Am Psychol. 1986, 41: 246-263.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Stokes R, Frederick-Recascino C: Women’s perceived body image: relations with personal happiness. J Women Aging. 2003, 15: 17-29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Crandall CS, Biernat M: The ideology of anti-fat attitudes. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1990, 20: 227-243. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1990.tb00408.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Crandall CS, Martinez R: Culture, ideology, and antifat attitudes. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1996, 22: 1165-1176. 10.1177/01461672962211007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Crandall CS, D’Anello S, Sakalli N, Lazarus E, Nejtardt GW, Feather NT: An attribution-value model of prejudice: anti-fat attitudes in six nations. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2001, 27: 30-37. 10.1177/0146167201271003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Stice E: Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2002, 128: 825-848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Thompson JK, Stice E: Thin-ideal internalization: mounting evidence for a new risk factor for body-image disturbance and eating pathology. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2001, 10: 181-183. 10.1111/1467-8721.00144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Grabe S, Ward LM, Hyde JS: The role of the media in body image concerns among women: a meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies. Psychol Bull. 2008, 134: 460-476.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Levine MP, Murnen SK: “Everybody knows that mass media are/are not [pick one] a cause of eating disorders”: a critical review of evidence for a causal link between media, negative body image, and disordered eating in females. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2009, 28: 9-42. 10.1521/jscp.2009.28.1.9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Stice E, Mazotti L, Krebs M, Martin S: Predictors of adolescent dieting behaviors: a longitudinal study. Psychol Addict Behav. 1998, 12: 195-205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Stice E, Shaw HE: Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and maintenance of eating pathology: a synthesis of research findings. J Psychosom Res. 2002, 53: 985-993. 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00488-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Swami V: Body appreciation, media influence, and weight status predict consideration of cosmetic surgery among female undergraduates. Body Image. 2009, 6: 315-317. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.07.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Twamley EW, Davis MC: The sociocultural model of eating disturbance in young women: the effects of personal attributes and family environment. J Soc Clin Psychol. 1999, 18: 467-489. 10.1521/jscp.1999.18.4.467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Warren CS, Schoen A, Schafer KJ: Media Internalization and Social Comparison as Predictors of Eating Pathology Among Latino Adolescents: The Moderating Effect of Gender and Generational Status. Sex Roles. 2010, 63: 712-724. 10.1007/s11199-010-9876-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Gardner RM, Stark K, Friedman BN, Jackson NA: Predictors of eating disorder scores in children ages 6 through 14: a longitudinal study. J Psychosom Res. 2000, 49: 199-205. 10.1016/S0022-3999(00)00172-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Cafri G, Yamamiya Y, Brannick M, Thompson JK, Florida S: The influence of sociocultural factors on body image: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2005, 12: 421-433. 10.1093/clipsy.bpi053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Homan K: Athletic-ideal and thin-ideal internalization as prospective predictors of body dissatisfaction, dieting, and compulsive exercise. Body Image. 2010, 7: 240-245. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.02.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Cahill S, Mussap AJ: Emotional reactions following exposure to idealized bodies predict unhealthy body change attitudes and behaviors in women and men. J Psychosom Res. 2007, 62: 631-639. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.11.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Homan K, McHugh E, Wells D, Watson C, King C: The effect of viewing ultra-fit images on college women’s body dissatisfaction. Body Image. 2012, 9: 50-56. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.07.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Groesz LM, Levine MP, Murnen SK: The effect of experimental presentation of thin media images on body satisfaction: a meta-analytic review. Int J Eat Disord. 2002, 31: 1-16. 10.1002/eat.10005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Calogero RM, Davis WN, Thompson JK: The Sociocultural Attitudes Toward Appearance Questionnaire (SATAQ-3): reliability and normative comparisons of eating disordered patients. Body Image. 2004, 1: 193-198. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.01.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Coughlin JW, Schreyer CC, Sarwer DB, Heinberg LJ, Redgrave GW, Guarda AS: Cosmetic surgery in inpatients with eating disorders: attitudes and experience. Body Image. 2012, 9: 180-183. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.10.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Rousseau A, Valls M, Chabrol H: Validation of the French version of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Scale-3 (SATAQ-3). kL'Encéphale. 2010, 36: 270-276.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Sánchez-Carracedo D, Barrada JR, López-Guimerà G, Fauquet J, Almenara CA, Trepat E: Analysis of the factor structure of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire (SATAQ-3) in Spanish secondary-school students through exploratory structural equation modeling. Body Image. 2012, 9: 163-171. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.10.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Thompson C, Russell-Mayhew S, Saraceni R: Evaluating the effects of a peer-support model: reducing negative body esteem and disordered eating attitudes and behaviours in grade eight girls. Eat Disord. 2012, 20: 113-126. 10.1080/10640266.2012.653946.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Wilksch SM, Wade TD: Reduction of shape and weight concern in young adolescents: a 30-month controlled evaluation of a media literacy program. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr. 2009, 48: 652-661. 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181a1f559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Karazsia BT, Crowther JH: Psychological and behavioral correlates of the SATAQ-3 with males. Body Image. 2008, 5: 109-115. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.08.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Suisman JL, O’Connor SM, Sperry S, Thompson JK, Keel PK, Burt SA, Neale M, Boker S, Sisk C, Klump KL: Genetic and environmental influences on thin-ideal internalization. Int J Eat Disord. 2012, 45: 1-7. 10.1002/eat.20900.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Hall EE: Baird S a, Gilbert DN, Miller PC, Bixby WR: Viewing television shows containing ideal and neutral body images while exercising: does type of body image content influence exercise performance and body image in women?. J Heal Psychol. 2011, 16: 938-946. 10.1177/1359105310396394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Lindsay AR, Warren CS, Velasquez SC, Lu M: A gender-specific approach to improving substance abuse treatment for women: The Healthy Steps to Freedom program. J Subst Abus Treat. 2012, 43: 61-9. 10.1016/j.jsat.2011.10.027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Wheeler DL, Vassar M, Hale WD: A gender-based measurement invariance study of the Sociocultural Attitudes Toward Appearance Questionnaire-3. Body image. 2011, 8: 168-172. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.12.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Franko DL, Jenkins A, Roehrig JP, Luce KH, Crowther JH, Rodgers RF: Psychometric properties of measures of eating disorder risk in Latina college women. Int J Eat Disord. 2012, 45: 592-596. 10.1002/eat.20979.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Cordero ED: Self-esteem, social support, collectivism, and the thin-ideal in Latina undergraduates. Body Image. 2011, 8: 82-85. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.11.006.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Grabe S, Hyde JS: Ethnicity and body dissatisfaction among women in the United States: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2006, 132: 622-640.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Poran MA: The politics of protection: body image, social pressures, and the misrepresentation of young black women. Sex Roles. 2006, 55: 739-755. 10.1007/s11199-006-9129-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Robinson TN, Killen JD, Litt IF, Hammer LD, Wilson DM, Haydel KF, Hayward C, Taylor CB: Ethnicity and body dissatisfaction: are Hispanic and Asian girls at increased risk for eating disorders?. J Adolesc Heal. 1996, 19: 384-393. 10.1016/S1054-139X(96)00087-0.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Nouri M, Hill LG, Orrell-Valente JK: Media exposure, internalization of the thin ideal, and body dissatisfaction: comparing Asian American and European American college females. Body Image. 2011, 8: 366-372. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.05.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Yates A, Edman J, Aruguete M: Ethnic differences in BMI and body/self-dissatisfaction among Whites, Asian subgroups, Pacific Islanders, and African-Americans. J Adolesc Heal. 2004, 34: 300-307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Kaiser HF: An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika. 1974, 39: 31-36. 10.1007/BF02291575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Lorenzo-Seva U, Berge JMF: Tucker’s congruence coefficient as a meaningful index of factor similarity. Methodology. 2006, 2: 57-64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Church AT, Burke PJ: Exploratory and confirmatory tests of the big five and Tellegen’s three- and four-dimensional models. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1994, 66: 93-114.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. MacCallum RC, Widaman KF, Zhang S, Hong S: Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological Methods. 1999, 4: 84-99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Llorente E, Warren CS, De Pérez Eulate L, Gleaves DH: A Spanish version of the Sociocultural Attitudes towards Appearance Questionnaire-3 (SATAQ-3): translation and psychometric evaluation. J Clin Psychol. 2013, 69: 240-251. 10.1002/jclp.21944.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Wilksch SM, Wade TD: Examination of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-3 in a mixed-gender young-adolescent sample. Psychol Assess. 2011, 24: 352-64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. WHO Technical Report Series: Physical status: The use and interpretation of anthropometry. 1995, World Health Organization: Geneva, 854-

    Google Scholar 

  52. Guadagnoli E, Velicer WF: Relation to sample size to the stability of component patterns. Psychol Bull. 1988, 103: 265-275.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Markland D, Oliver EJ: The Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-3: a confirmatory factor analysis. Body Image. 2008, 5: 116-21. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.10.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Overstreet NM, Quinn DM, Agocha VB: Beyond Thinness: The Influence of a Curvaceous Body Ideal on Body Dissatisfaction in Black and White Women. Sex Roles. 2010, 63: 91-103. 10.1007/s11199-010-9792-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Miller KJ, Gleaves DH, Hirsch TG, Green BA, Snow AC, Corbett CC: Comparisons of body image dimensions by race/ethnicity and gender in a university population. Int J Eat Disord. 2000, 27: 310-316. 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(200004)27:3<310::AID-EAT8>3.0.CO;2-Q.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Rubin LR, Fitts ML, Becker AE: “Whatever feels good in my soul”: body ethics and aesthetics among African American and Latina women. Cult Med Psychiatr. 2003, 27: 49-75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Schaefer LM, Thompson JK, Heinberg LJ, Calogero RM, Nerini A, Dittmar H, Swami V, Stefanile C, Clark L, Adams Z, Macwana SPS: Validation of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-4 (SATAQ-4) in Italian, British, and Australian Women. 2012, Austin, TX: In Annual Academy of Eating Disorders - International Conference on Eating Disorders (ICED)

    Google Scholar 

  58. Warren CS: Body area dissatisfaction in white, black and Latina female college students in the USA: an examination of racially salient appearance areas and ethnic identity. Ethnic and Racial Studies. 2012, 35: 1-20. 10.1080/01419870.2011.594177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Falconer JW, Neville HA: African American College Women’s Body Image. Psychol Women Q. 2000, 24: 236-243. 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb00205.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cortney S Warren.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

CSW designed the study, contributed to drafting of the manuscript and performed initial data analyses. DHG edited the manuscript and performed final data analyses. LMR helped write the manuscript and managed the dataset. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Warren, C.S., Gleaves, D.H. & Rakhkovskaya, L.M. Score reliability and factor similarity of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-3 (SATAQ-3) among four ethnic groups. J Eat Disord 1, 14 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-2974-1-14

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-2974-1-14

Keywords